Showing posts with label quantitative. Show all posts
Showing posts with label quantitative. Show all posts

Tuesday, April 9, 2013

Which Way to Compare: Part 2 – Why Percentage Distributions Are Also Better Than Indexing

Some of our clients are used to seeing comparative data presented in the form of an index – a standardized score set to 100. Their own score for the indexed item is then shown relative to the index. For example, if the national norm on a survey item is a score of 60, then 60 is indexed to 100. If the client’s score on that item is s72, that is 20% higher than 60, producing a relative score of 120.

With an index, data users can see at a glance how they stand relative to the comparative norm. This way of showing a comparison is conceptually easy to grasp and is common in types of research where the focus is heavily on the relative strength of whatever is being measured in comparison to some standard. Certain types of advertising research, in particular, uses indexing. When a proposed new ad is being tested, for example, the emphasis is not just on how well the ad does with its intended audience, but also on how well it does relative to other ads. A successful ad has to break through media clutter and there is no benefit in producing an ad and making an expensive media buy only to have the ad fail to stand out against the noise of other ads.

The strength of using an index, however, is also its weakness. By its very nature, an index puts the analytic focus on the comparison, not on whether the actual score is high or low, good, bad or indifferent. An example can help make this point. Imagine that we are designing a dessert menu for a restaurant and they survey their patrons and ask “Do you like chocolate flavored desserts?” We find that against the industry norm, the restaurant’s patrons index below the norm - only 90 against the index of 100. We might be tempted to limit the presence of chocolate in the dessert menu in favor of other flavors. An examination of the actual percentages, however, is more revealing. The norm shows that 80% of the population like chocolate desserts, while the restaurant patrons’ score was 72%. So despite the low index score, this is still a substantial majority of the folks dining at the restaurant and we would certainly want to make sure there are some chocolate goodies on that dessert menu.

As in the example above, using an index can be misleading. This is especially true if the index represents a very high percentage (like the example above) or a very low percentage. A client who “beats” an index based on a low score may feel proud, even though there is little to brag about, and may feel no changes or improvements are needed even when there is plenty of room to do so. Similarly, failing to match an index based on a very high percentage can cause clients to be upset or start wasting time and resources fixing a problem that isn’t really much of a problem at all.

So while we do feel that indexing has its place – wherever comparison is the whole point of the exercise – we think that it’s generally better to know a little bit more of the details. Most research is about making decisions and setting priorities that are about much more than whether or not you have outscored an index. A percentage distribution compared to a well-constructed norm lets data users really see where they stand, set goals that are achievable and meaningful, and take pride in legitimate successes.

Thursday, March 28, 2013

Which Way to Compare? Part 1 – Why Percentage Distributions are Better than Averages

Our work for our clients, especially our employee satisfaction and engagement studies, often includes comparisons to national or industry norms or across groups within their organizations. These comparisons enable clients to where they stand in and help them set reasonable goals for organizational improvement. In recent weeks we’ve had several conversations regarding the pros and cons of different ways of expressing these comparative figures – as percentage distributions, as averages and as indexes. We strongly feel that percentage distributions offer the best approach in most cases. Today we’ll show why we prefer percentage distributions over averages and in the next blog we’ll show why we also prefer percentages over indexes.

Averages offer the benefit of simplicity for the end users of data. If a survey question has a 5-point scale that is converted to the numbers 1 through 5, taking the numerical average of the responses produces a score between 1 and 5. It’s then a simple matter to compare across groups. If we put the “5” at the positive end of the scale, then those groups – workgroups, locations, divisions – with higher scores are doing better than those whose scores are lower. It’s easy to glance at a set of these average scores and identify priorities for improvement.

The problem with using averages, however, lies in the nature of the average (technically known as the arithmetic mean), as a statistic. An average is a measure of central tendency and has an underlying assumption that the answers are more-or-less normally distributed. This assumption is often incorrect. It is not uncommon to find survey responses that are skewed toward one end of the scale or even polarized. Using a central tendency measure when there is no central tendency can reduce the utility of the information or even be misleading. A simple example can show why this is true. Imagine three work groups all answering the question “How much do you like your job?” using s 5-point scale. Each group has 10 employees:

In group one, all 10 employees choose the middle of the scale

In group two, 5 employees choose one end of the scale, and 5 choose the other end

In group three, 2 employees choose each of the 5 points on the scale

The average score for all three groups is a “3.” None of these groups has a central tendency and taking an average obscures an important feature of the data – the way the opinions are distributed. If these three work groups all reported to you, which information would be most actionable – knowing that they all have the same average score or knowing something about how the scores are distributed? We think the answer is pretty obvious.

Whether in market research or national politics the difference between winning and losing is often in the percentage distribution, not the average. In the 2012 election, more votes were cast for Democratic candidates for the House of Representatives than for Republican candidates, but we have a Republican majority in the House because of the way those votes were distributed across congressional districts. Nate Silver made his reputation as a predictor of elections by understanding the details of percentage distributions of voter behavior. We feel our clients need and deserve the same level of information about the issues that are important to them. So even though average scores are easy to calculate and present, we think that looking at percentage distributions is worth it.

Wednesday, August 22, 2012

The Perception Dilemma, Or, What Can We Do About Self-Report Bias?

A recent article in the Sunday New York Times called “Why Waiting Is Torture” (http://www.nytimes.com/2012/08/19/opinion/sunday/why-waiting-in-line-is-torture.html?pagewanted=all) brought to mind one of key dilemmas in survey design – the simple fact that people often “misremember” their experiences (which is what we call “self-report bias”). How reliable can survey results be if respondents cannot accurately recall what happened?

The article itself is about the psychology of waiting in lines and some of the points are very interesting (although perhaps not surprising to researchers!):

1. According to Richard Larson at M.I.T., occupied time (such as walking to a specific location) feels shorter than unoccupied time (such as standing around waiting),

2. There is a tendency to overestimate the amount of time spent waiting in line (the article quotes an average of 36%),

3. A sense of uncertainty, such as not knowing how long you will be in line, increases the stress of waiting, while information and feedback on wait times or reasons for delays improve perceptions,

4. When there are multiple lines, customers focus on the lines they are “losing to” and not on the lines they are beating, and

5. The frustrations of waiting can be mitigated in the final moments by beating expectations, such as having the line suddenly speed up.

What implications do these findings have on survey design and analysis? In my experience, if we are trying to get an accurate record of an event – such as the amount of time waiting in line – a straightforward recall question is not always the best choice. There are actions we can take during research design, in developing our data collection tools and in analysis to deal with the problems or poor or inaccurate self-report of behavior.

At the research design stage, we should ask whether a self-report on a survey question is the best way to collect the data. In some cases, we are better off using direct measures, such as observations of the behavior, instead of asking about it. At the questionnaire development stage, we can explore which ways of asking a question are more likely to limit bias, for example asking people what hours they watched TV last night will produce a larger per night (and more accurate) answer than asking people to estimate their total viewing hours per week. In the analysis stage we often know which direction the self-report bias will tend to lean – for example, people generally under-report their consumption of alcohol and over-report their church attendance. When we know these tendencies we can deal with them either by adjusting the answers up or down – if we know the appropriate adjustment to make – or by mentioning them when we report the findings or make recommendations.

The key here is to take the possibility of self-report bias into consideration and to have a plan for dealing with it. The existence of self-report bias does not invalidate research efforts, it is merely one of the many factors that research vendors and clients must take into consideration as they approach their projects.

Tuesday, April 24, 2012

Getting the Best Value from Open-Ended Questions

An article by Carolyn Lindquist called “For Better Insights From Text Analytics, Elicit Better Comments” in the most recent edition of Quirks Marketing Research (April 2012) gives three recommendations for improving the quality of consumer responses to open-ended questions. These three recommendations are:

1. Target your questions

2. Ask why

3. Be sensitive to placement

Based on my own experience, these are worthwhile considerations when designing surveys. I think most quantitative researchers – including me! – can fall into the twin traps of asking too many open-ends in a single survey and not defining those open-ends as clearly as possible.

I’m a strong believer in what I consider “directed open-ends,” which means that the wording is specific to the situation rather than a catch-all “please list comments below.” For example, in concept tests, I strongly believe in asking for strengths and weaknesses separately and this makes the survey both easier to answer and to analyze. This is consistently with Carolyn’s recommendation to “target your questions” – the example she gives is to link is to vary the open-ended question text according to the stated level of overall satisfaction.

I’m intrigued by Carolyn’s suggestion to ask “why” rather than “what questions,” as they have found that asking “why” (such as “please tell us why you were less than satisfied with your experience”) yields longer and more useful answers than asking “what” (as in “please tell us what we can do to improve your next experience”). She has found that the responses to “what” questions contain less detail and emotion than the answers to “why” questions. I think this suggestion is worth testing out. However, this does not mean we should ask “why” after every rating question, as we’ve had some clients request a few times over the years!

I also agree with her third recommendation on being sensitive to the placement of open-ended questions, although I don’t agree with her suggestion that open-ends should only be asked at the end of a survey. In my experience, open-ended questions should appear where they make the most sense in a survey and a nice balance of quantitative rating questions and open-ends makes for a more pleasant and natural survey-taking experience. One caveat though – I avoid having too many open-ended questions listed sequentially, as I believe that too many open-ends in a row can lead to a feeling that the survey is longer than it actually is and lead to respondent fatigue.

Thursday, April 19, 2012

Some Practical Advice on Statistical Testing

One thing that I am willing to admit is that I am a very “practical” researcher, meaning that I prefer to rely on the craft of analysis when constructing a story more than statistical analyses. This is not to say that advanced statistical tools do not have their place within a researcher’s tool box but they should not substitute for the attention required to carefully review the results and to dig deep through cross tabs to uncover the patterns in the data so as to create the a relevant and meaningful story. Remember the adage – “the numbers don’t speak for themselves, the researcher has to speak for the numbers.”
A great example of this is the use – and misuse – of statistical testing. I would never claim to be a statistician but, over the years, I’ve found that the type of statistical testing that often accompanies data analysis has very limited uses. In a nutshell, statistical testing is great for warning analysts when apparent differences in percentages are not significantly different. This is extremely important when deciding what action to take based on the results. However, such testing is no use on its own when determining whether statistically significant differences are meaningful. In my experience, statistical significance works as a good test of difference but such differences alone are insufficient when analyzing research data.
I love this comment from an article by Connie Schmitz on the use of statistics in surgical education and research that “Statistical analysis has a narrative role to play in our work. But to tell a good story, it has to make sense.” (http://www.facs.org/education/rap/schmitz0207.html) She points out that, with a large enough sample size, every comparison between findings can be labeled “significant,” as well as concluding that “it is particularly difficult to determine the importance of findings if one cannot translate statistical results back into the instrument’s original units of measure, into English, and then into practical terms.”
The idea of translating survey results into practical terms represents the very foundation of what I believe market research should be doing. This same idea is highlighted in an article by Terry Grapentine in the April 2011 edition of Quirks Marketing Research called “Statistical Significance Revisited.” Building on an even earlier Quirk’s article from 1994 called “The Use, Misuse and Abuse of Significance” (http://www.quirks.com/articles/a1994/19941101.aspx?searchID=29151901), he stresses that statistical testing does not render a verdict on the validity of the data being analyzed. He highlights examples of both sampling error and measurement error that can have major impacts on the validity of survey results that would not at all affect the decision that a particular difference is “statistically significant.” I agree wholeheartedly with his conclusion that “unfortunately, when one includes the results of statistical tests in a report, doing so confers a kind of specious statement on a study’s ‘scientific’ precision and validity” while going on to point out that “precision and validity are not the same thing.”
Personally, I find it especially frustrating when research analysis is limited to pointing out each and every one of the statistically significant differences, with the reader expected to draw their own conclusions from this laundry list of differences. How can that possibly be helpful in deciding what action to take? In this case, the researcher has simply failed to fulfill one of their key functions – describing the results in a succinct, coherent and relevant manner. In contrast, I believe that I follow the recommendation of Terry Grapentine (and of Patrick Baldasare and Vikas Mittel before him) that researchers should be seeking and reporting on “managerial significance,” by focusing on the differences in survey results “whose magnitude have relevance to decision making.” This is quite a different approach than simply reciting back the results that are statistically different.
Going back to Connie Schmitz’s article, she closes with a great observation conveyed by Geoffrey Norman and David Streiner in their book PDQ Statistics:
“Always keep in mind the advice of Winifred Castle, a British statistician, who wrote that, ‘We researchers use statistics the way a drunkard uses a lamp post, more for support than illumination’.”

Tuesday, April 17, 2012

The Risks of Projecting Survey Results To A Larger Population

In my experience, most quantitative research results are analyzed on the basis of the survey results themselves – such as the percentage distributions on rating scales – without the need to project results onto the larger population that the sample represents. It is generally understood that, with reasonably rigorous sampling procedures, these distributions are reflective of the attitudes held by the population at large.

In some instances, though, it is important to project to the larger group, such as when creating estimates of product use based on concept results. In these cases, we face a special challenge – do we take consumers at their word and simply extrapolate their answers to the larger population or do we use some combination of common sense and experience to adjust the data?

Although there are many sophisticated models for translating interest in a new product or service into projections of first year use, most include “adjustments” to the survey data to account for typical consumer behavior, such as:

1. The typical 5-point purchase intent scale is weighted in order to more accurately predict what proportion of the population will actually try the product. For example, the proportion of those who would “definitely buy” might be given a weight of 80% to reflect a high, but not absolute, likelihood of buying whereas those who would “probably buy” might be given a weight of just 20%.

2. Secondly, these results assume 100% awareness of the new product or service so further adjustments are required to account for the anticipated build in awareness, usually as a result of advertising, and

3. Thirdly, some estimate of repeat purchase is required, often derived from consumer experience with the new product or service or from established market results.

We take these steps to mitigate the risk of simply applying the survey results to the total population, as this could wildly inflate potential use of a new product or service.

This issue came to my mind this weekend when reading a New York Times article called “The Cybercrime Wave That Wasn’t” (http://www.nytimes.com/2012/04/15/opinion/sunday/the-cybercrime-wave-that-wasnt.html) in which Dinei Florêncio and Cormac Herley of Microsoft Research conclude that, although some cybercriminals may do well, “cybercrime is a relentless, low-profit struggle for the majority.”

Part of their analysis questions the highly-touted estimates of the value of cybercrime, including a recent claim of annual losses among consumers at $114 billion worldwide. This estimate makes the value of such crime comparable to estimates of the global drug trade. As it turns out, however, Florêncio and Herley conclude that “such widely circulated cybercrime estimates are generated using absurdly bad statistical methods, making them wholly unreliable.” This is a very practical example of how results from what appear to be reasonably large research samples can run into critical problems of statistical reliability, whether through poor sampling, naïve extrapolation or other sorts of statistical errors. In the case of the cybercrime estimate, it appears that the estimates of losses that come from just 1 or 2 people in the research sample are being extrapolated to the entire population, which means that

In this particular example, a more accurate approach would be to separate the “screening” sample – i.e., identifying those consumers who have been victims of cybercrime using an extremely large database – from the “outcome” sample. In other words, if the goal is to estimate the impact of cybercrime, the objective should be to find a reliable sample of victims and interview them on their experience, including the extent of their losses. This approach would provide a much more rigorous basis for estimating the total value of cybercrime. However, caution should still be exercised when projecting to the total population.

The key learning is that anytime we have data we want to extrapolate, we need to think about how much we trust that data to be accurate. There are some things consumers can report with superb accuracy - where they ate lunch today, the size of their mortgage payment, how many pets are in their homes. Assuming a decent survey sample, data of this sort can be easily extrapolated to a larger population. But other kinds of data are less accurate, whether due to the limits of human recall or various other forms of bias. Studies have shown, for example, that survey respondents cannot accurately recall where they ate lunch a week or two ago (recall error), tend to under-report their alcohol consumption (social desirability bias) and over-estimate their future purchases of products we show them in concept tests.

So, if we wish to extrapolate from our survey data to a larger sample, we have to be honest about how accurate the results are, what sorts of bias might inflate or deflate the numbers, and what sorts of adjustments, if any, we should make. And when we see stories in the media with giant estimates of the prevalence of some sort of crime, social problem or behavioral trend, we need to take a moment to ask how they came up with those numbers. Often, with a little digging, we see problems in how these estimates were created, leading to the same need for logic and common sense that we find when dealing with our own market projections.

Monday, April 9, 2012

How and When Should I Use Statistical Testing?

Statistical testing is a common deliverable provided by market research vendors. But in some cases the users of the research findings may be uncertain about what the statistical testing really means and whether or not it should influence the way they use the data. Below are five key questions to keep in mind when using statistical testing.
1. What kind of data am I dealing with? Statistical testing can only be applied to quantitative data, such as survey data. There are no statistical tests for qualitative data, such as focus groups and in-depth interviews.
2. What am I trying to learn? Most statistical testing is used primarily to help decide which of the differences we see in our data are real in terms of the population we are interested in. For example, if your findings show that 45% of men like a new product concept and 55% of women like the concept, you need to decide if that difference is real ‒that is, the difference seen in your survey accurately reflects a difference between men and women that exists in the larger population of target consumers.
3. How certain do I need to be? Confidence intervals are the most common way of deciding whether percentage differences of this sort are meaningful. The size of a confidence interval is determined by the level of certainty we demand – usually 90% or 95% in market research, 95% or 99% in medical research – and the size of our sample relative to the population it is drawn from. The higher the level of certainty we demand, the wider the confidence interval will be – with a very high standard of certainty, we need a wide interval to be sure we have captured the true population percentage. Conversely, the bigger the sample, the narrower the confidence interval - as the sample gets bigger it becomes more and more like the target population and we become more certain that the differences we see are valid.
4. How good is my sample? Most statistical tests rely on key assumptions about how you selected the sample of people from whom you collected your data. For tests like the confidence intervals described above, this key assumption is having some element of random selection built into your sample that makes it mathematically representative of the population you are studying. The further your sampling procedure strays from this assumption, the less valid your statistical testing will be. If you can make the case that your sample is not biased in any important ways relevant to your research questions, you can rely on your stats tests to identify meaningful differences. If you have doubts about your sample, use the tests with caution.
5. Does my data meet other key assumptions about the test? Some stats tests assume particular data distributions, such as the bell-shaped curve which is an underlying assumption for confidence intervals. If your data are distributed in some other way – lop-sided toward the high or low end of the scale or polarized – the stats test is worse than worthless, it will actually be misleading!
6. Does the stats testing seem to align with other things I know about the research topic? Stats tests should supplement your overall understanding of the data. They are not a substitute for common sense. Keep in mind that most data analysis software will produce stats tests automatically, whether or not the tests are appropriate for the particular data set you are using. Almost every experienced researcher has watched someone (or been someone) trying to explain a “finding” that was nothing more than a meaningless software output.
If you can provide honest, satisfactory answers to these five questions, stats testing can hugely improve your understanding of your data and help you identify its key themes. And likewise, these key questions can keep you from wasting your time analyzing differences that aren’t really there.

Tuesday, January 24, 2012

How Many People Do I Need To Survey To Get Meaningful Answers?

A central decision for anyone considering a survey – or any other quantitative research – is figuring out how big the survey sample needs to be in order to produce meaningful answers to the research questions. Researchers focus on sample size because it ties together three core aspects of any research effort:

  • Cost – the bigger the sample, the more it will cost to collect, process and analyze the data
  • Speed – the bigger the sample, the longer it will take to collect it (big samples can sometimes be collected quickly, but usually only by further raising costs!)
  • Accuracy – the bigger the sample, the more certain we can be that we have correctly captured the perceptions/opinions/behavior/beliefs/feelings of the population we are interested in (the technical term for this is statistical reliability)

As we see from these three bullets, the decision about sample size essentially boils down to a trade-off between cost, speed and accuracy. So when we pick a sample size we are making a decision about how much accuracy we are going to purchase, within the framework of our budget and timing.

Fortunately for researchers, quantitative samples do not have to be enormous to provide findings that are accurate enough to answer most market research questions. Any unbiased sample (we’ll talk about sample bias in another blog entry) of 50 or more stands a halfway decent chance of giving you a reasonable view of the population it is drawn from and, as we increase the sample size, our confidence that we have the correct answer increases. We can show this effect by looking at the margin of error – the plus or minus number – for some common sample sizes. To keep it simple, all of these are calculated using the assumption that the sample is drawn from a large population (20,000 or more) and that we are using the 95% confidence level of statistical reliability (the most typical standard for statistical reliability used in market research). If we are looking at percentages:

  • A sample of 100 has a margin of error of ± 9.8%
  • A sample of 250 has a margin of error of ± 6.2%
  • A sample of 500 has a margin of error of ± 4.4%
  • A sample of 1,000 has a margin of error of ± 3.0%
  • A sample of 2,000 has a margin of error of ± 2.1%

Looking at these numbers you can see why national surveys, such as the big public opinion polls shown on TV or in newspapers, often have samples around 1,000 or so. Samples in that size range have small margins of error, and doubling the sample size wouldn’t make the margin of error much smaller – there’s no reason to spend money making the sample bigger for such a small gain in accuracy.

These numbers also show why we often urge clients to spend a bit to make a small sample bigger, but not too big! The gains in accuracy are all at the beginning – moving from a sample of 100 to something larger is almost always a good idea, while adding anything over 1,000 usually is not. So the rule of thumb is: 100 is probably too small and 1,000 is probably too big.

Of course, in real life it can be more complicated. We may need to examine sub-groups (age or income groups, political parties, geographic regions, etc.) within the population we are looking at. If a sub-group is small, we may need a bigger overall sample to capture enough of each of the sub-groups in order to provide an accurate picture of their views. So we have a rule of thumb about sub-groups, as well – don’t make decisions about any sub-group smaller than 30. For example, if we do a survey of households in a large urban area and we want to compare households by income level, we need to make our sample big enough to have at least 30 households in each of the income categories we want to compare. Assuming this is a normal city, there will be fewer households at the high end of the income distribution than at the low end, so we need to think about how to get enough of the high-end households to be able to make how comparison. So, if we want to be able to look at households with income over $100K, and 15% of the population has an income of $100K more, we need to have a sample of at least 200 households to ensure that 30 of the households would be in that category.

Using these rules of thumb, you can form an idea about how big your sample needs to be to answer your research questions - without spending more than you can afford.

Friday, January 13, 2012

The “Dirty Dozen” – The Most Common Things That Cause Market Research To Go Wrong

Clients who are new to market research often worry that they will spend time and money on a market research project only to later realize that errors in the design or execution of the study have rendered their investment much less valuable than they had hoped. And they are right to worry – there are plenty of cases of market research blunders and even examples where market research findings have led to product design or marketing decisions that were worse than if there had been no research at all!
There are lots of individual events or mistakes that can knock market research projects off track and a short blog entry could never list them all. But there are a few types of errors that account for most of the problems. We think of these as the “dirty dozen,” – the common mistakes that we see over and over again and that rob market research studies of their potential value to decision makers. The tables below list these problems by the stage of the research project where they typically occur, along with the consequences the problem brings and – most importantly – ways to avoid them.
At the research design stage:
Problem Consequences Solution
Poorly formulated objectives or research questions The data collected will not address the real issues, resulting in findings that are vague, inconclusive or even misleading. Write out your objectives and research questions and think about what kind of data would count as an answer to each one. Don’t skip this step or assume that everybody on the project has a shared understanding of what the research is supposed to accomplish.
Poor choice of data collection method(s) Lack of insight when needed depth or breadth (or sometimes both!) is missing from the collected data. Make the method(s) suit the objectives and research questions. Don’t get locked into “standard” approaches or doing what is easy rather than what is best for the study.
Sample design issues Asking questions of the wrong people can produce misleading answers in qualitative studies or statistically invalidate a quantitative project. Be explicit about the sample parameters. Know who you are going to talk to and exactly what larger population the sample is supposed to represent.
Poorly designed/untested research instrument Garbage in – garbage out. Failing to ask well-though-out questions, whether in a survey, a focus group or an in-depth interview, will produce poor quality results. Every question you ask should have a purpose that relates back to the research objectives. Don’t neglect review and testing of the questionnaire or interview guide.
At the data collection stage:
Problem Consequences Solution
Inadequately trained or prepared data collectors Data that is inconsistently gathered can produce gaps, validity problems and lack of depth. Use professionals who know their jobs and have proven track records. Even experienced survey data collectors, interviewers and focus group moderators need to practice with the research instrument.
Failure to meet the sample specifications If the sample you get is not the sample you intended, you may have data that is not pertinent or that misrepresents the views of the target population. Have good quality control on the sample. If adjustments have to be made, be very sure you are not giving up the validity of your sample in order to fill your groups or meet numerical quotas.
Quality control issues Poor quality control can result in errors in the data files, data that is missing or is mis- categorized. Have a plan to check incoming data as it is collected. Don’t wait until data collection is over to begin the process of checking for errors or problems.
Loss of data You can’t analyze data that has disappeared. Have back-ups (and more-back-ups). Never let data reside longer than necessary in a single location or file. Have security and back-up procedures for all data storage media.
At the data analysis/interpretation stage:
Problem Consequences Solution
Lazy/incomplete review of the raw data Important insights can be missed. Have a data analysis plan that sets out how the raw data will be handled. Allow enough time for data review and processing. Don’t rely on human memory or quick skimming to capture all the meaning that the data holds.
Inappropriate data reduction techniques Each time data is reduced, whether through coding of qualitative data or numerical consolidation of quantitative data, there is some potential loss of information or important details. Make sure your chosen data reduction techniques capture the themes, ideas and categories that will answer the research questions. Don’t be afraid to recode or re-analyze if new issues emerge while data reduction is in progress. Remember recoding means you have learned something from your data – it’s a step forward not a step back!
Over-reliance on statistics Interpretation that is guided only by statistical testing runs the risk of missing insights that didn’t quite pass the test criteria or finding “insights” that are really just an artifact of the statistical method. Know the strengths and weaknesses of the statistics you use. Use them as guidelines and tools, not as the word of the research gods. Statistics are not a substitute for common sense of familiarity with your data and your research topic.
Canned answers Having a bias toward a particular answer or type of interpretation can blind you to new themes and ideas that emerge from your data. Keep an open mind. Let the data speak to you. Think about what would count as disproof of your preferred interpretation and make sure that there’s a way for that evidence to emerge.
As you can see, there are many potential pitfalls and room for error in market research projects of any type. Careful planning, design, oversight and analysis are absolutely key to getting the best value from the money you spend on market research!

Friday, January 6, 2012

How Do Qualitative and Quantitative Research Differ?

For market researchers, the distinctions between qualitative and quantitative research are very clear, even to the point where many researchers specialize in one or the other of these two types. If the difference is so clear in our minds, then why is it so difficult to write a clear, straightforward, practical and meaningful explanation of the difference?
Most explanations of the qualitative/quantitative distinction tend to focus on listing the typical methods used for each. But this puts the emphasis on the means – the way the data is collected. The more important issue in thinking about the qualitative/quantitative distinction is on the ends. It is the type of research questions you are trying to answer, and more importantly, what type of information would count as a meaningful answer, that determines whether your approach should be qualitative, quantitative or a mix of both.
For example, if your research question is about how some behavior or opinion is distributed across different groups of people, geographic areas or across time, you need numbers – usually percentages – to make these comparisons. This sort of numerical comparison implies a quantitative approach and all the methodological trappings that go with it, including:
  • Closed-end survey-type questions
  • Relatively large numbers of research participants or direct observations
  • A sampling approach that ensures that you have the statistical validity to project the findings from your sample onto the population you are interested in
In contrast, if your research questions are less about comparisons and more about understanding chains of logic, decision processes, group norms, reactions to ideas or sensory stimuli, or cultural domains, you probably need a qualitative approach. Qualitative research focuses on understanding the human experience in context by either:
  • Directly embedding the research in the scene or cultural setting (ethnography)
  • Using group dynamics to explore norms, attitudes, reactions and beliefs (focus groups)
  • Employing inductive questioning to probe reasoning, causation and psychology (in-depth interviews)
In short, it is knowing both the research question and what counts as an answer to the question that determines which “toolkit” the researcher will turn to for a specific project.
That said, the stage of our knowledge about a topic is often an important factor. When we first begin learning about a new topic we are usually in an exploratory mode. We may still be developing our vocabulary about the topic and couldn’t write a good survey about it if we tried – we don’t know enough to write good closed-end questions! Qualitative research is often used at this stage. The very “hands-on” qualitative techniques enable us to learn the lay of the land – how consumers think and speak about the topic. When we see research objectives using verbs like explore or identify – we are usually at this stage of knowledge and lean toward qualitative techniques. We may also use qualitative research at a later stage of knowledge to help interpret quantitative data. In these cases we use interviews or focus groups to understand the correlations that appear in survey data.
Quantitative studies are usually done only in areas where we already have the appropriate knowledge and vocabulary to design good survey questions. We have to know enough about what to ask about, how to ask it, and who to ask it of, to meaningfully quantify the responses. Research objectives that include words such as test, measure or evaluate are more typical of this stage of knowledge.
In short, thinking about qualitative and quantitative research is not about choosing methods or techniques – it is about understanding the nature of your research questions and having a good sense of what type of data – percentages, narratives, images, word counts, observations, maps of logic chains or decision processes would provide useful answers. If you are clear about what you are asking and what you need to know, the choice of methods will largely take care of itself.

Friday, December 23, 2011

How Should Market Research Be Used?


In its simplest form, market research is a decision making tool. Its primary goal is to enable more informed choices on a wide range of topics, especially for such key issues as branding, positioning, new product/service development, product use, segmentation, consumer attitudes and customer experience or behavior.

Researchers have developed a large number of tools and approaches to help answer the questions that marketers have. The two biggest “categories” are qualitative research and quantitative research. We’ll go into more depth on each of these in future posts but, basically, qualitative research is generally used to explore a topic in depth. Focus groups are the most well-known form of qualitative research, although one-on-one interviews, Laddering interviews, ethnographic interviews and “mini groups” are also often used.

Qualitative research relies on only a small number of participants, meaning that the results are not strictly “projectable” to the population as a whole. However, qualitative research is great at uncovering the linkages between consumer attitudes and behavior and really understanding how people feel about a particular topic. It can be used as the first step toward designing a more comprehensive quantitative survey or can help explain or further explore survey results that have already been collected.

Quantitative research can best be summarized as a measurement tool, with the results usually gathered through the use of a survey. Survey data can be collected in a wide variety of ways, such as online, by telephone, by mail and even in person. The two most critical factors when designing a quantitative survey are (1) sample definition, that is, who is being interviewed and (2) survey design, including the approach to be taken and what questions are being asked. We’ll delve into both of these topics in later postings.

Market research is most powerful when marketers have clearly defined their objectives. When defining a project, the decision makers don’t need to focus on the research approach, the discussion guide or the detailed survey questions they just need to be certain about what they want to know. An experienced research can then take those objectives and create a study design to meet them. It’s really, really important for everyone involved to keep the study objectives front-and-center through all stages of any research project!